Is the Dark Triad Really Attractive? A Review of the Literature

Are Dark Triad individuals attractive or do they compensate with effective short-term strategies?

Enjoy DatePsychology? Consider subscribing at Patreon to support the project.

Follow my YouTube series reviewing every chapter in the Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology and Romantic Relationships.

Over the last decade, a “Dark Seduction” ecosystem of discourse has been produced by dating gurus and coaches. These refer back to the Dark Triad: a personality construct that consists of three traits (narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy) and is characterized by manipulativeness, lack of empathy, and a focus on self-interest.

A lot of research has been done on the Dark Triad, human mating, and attractiveness. Broadly across the literature, the Dark Triad has been associated with higher short-term mating orientation and success (Jonason et al., 2009; Carter et al., 2014). A key piece of evidence in support of higher mating success for individuals high in the DT is that they self-report having more sexual partners on average (Jonason et al., 2009; Borráz-León & Rantala, 2021).

However, having more sexual partners is not in itself evidence of higher attractiveness or desirability. The Dark Triad could be associated with more sexual activity because it is appealing, but being more appealing is not implied by having more sexual partners.

For example, Dark Triad individuals have lower standards in mate selection (Jonason et al., 2011) and lower sensitivity to sexual disgust (Burtăverde et al., 2021). When exposed to “dealbreakers,” individuals high in the Dark Triad are also less likely to express a change in their desire for someone as a romantic partner (Jonason et al., 2020). Dyadic mate value is highly correlated; individuals high in attractiveness tend to also select others high in attractiveness (Conroy-Beam et al., 2019). Having lower standards may simply open up the pool of potential mates. In other words, someone who is willing to have sex with pretty much anyone available will have more sexual partners on average, but this is not a testament to their own desirability or attractiveness.

Research robustly shows that people high in the Dark Triad have a stronger orientation toward short-term mating (Jonason et al., 2009, 2010, 2011; Carter et al., 2014; Koladich & Atkinson, 2016; Burtăverde et al., 2021; Kay, 2021) and a lower orientation toward long-term relationships (Jonason et al., 2012). High Dark Triad individuals are also higher in sociosexuality, or the willingness to pursue casual sex and multiple sexual partners, on average (Jonason et al., 2009). In large European and Latin American samples, both sociosexuality and Dark Triad traits were the highest in participants who exhibited high mating effort, but low investment in long-term relationships and parenting (Valentova et al., 2020). The Dark Triad may be a partial expression of a “fast life-history strategy:” a willingness to select potential mates indiscriminately and low commitment or investment in long-term relationships (Jonason et al., 2010, 2017; McDonald et al., 2012).

This is important to understand: high average desirability is not the only (nor even the best hypothesis) for having more lifetime sexual partners. For example, my meta-analysis on physical attractiveness and sexual partner count found only a weak relationship between the two (see: Male Attractiveness and Sexual Partner Count). Imagine two men who are of equally high desirability across measurements. The first man is picky and has a high long-term mating orientation; he finds an attractive woman and marries her. The second man is indiscriminate, has a strong desire to have sex with many women, and has little desire or ability to maintain a long-term relationship. The second man will accumulate more sexual partners over his lifespan, not because he is more desirable, but simply because that is what he wanted to do.

Results from past research are mixed on the relationship between the Dark Triad and attractiveness (we will review this below), but are more consistent when examining the behaviors of individuals high in the Dark Triad as short-term mating strategists. As described above, people high in the Dark Triad simply want to have more short-term sex. They are also more likely to use mating strategies that facilitate this.

Individuals high in the Dark Triad are more likely to engage in sexual coercion (Figueredo et al., 2015; Jonason et al., 2017; Prusik et al., 2021). They are more likely to commit sexual assault (Navas et al., 2022) and sexual harassment (Zeigler-Hill et al., 2016). Further, individuals high in the Dark Triad are more aware of potential victims (Zeigler-Hill et al., 2016). They are more likely to have a “whatever-it-takes” attitude to their social interactions more broadly (Jonason et al., 2012). Evolutionary scholars have proposed that the Dark Triad may be an “adaptation for sexual exploitation” (Jonason et al., 2017; see also Thornhill & Palmer, 2001). Consistent with this, individuals high in the Dark Triad are also more likely to be perceived by others as having a higher likelihood to commit rape (Pavlović et al., 2019). 

The Dark Triad is also associated with higher mating effort (Westhead & Egan, 2015) and more attempts at self-enhancement (Schröder–Abé et al., 2016). In other words, high Dark Triad individuals have more sexual partners because they try to have more sexual partners. Using 130 items from the Mate Attraction Scale (Buss, 1988), Monteiro et al. (2017) found that participants high in the Dark Triad were more likely to score high on items indicating mating effort, including being more likely to approach women. Rather than desirable men hanging back and being approached, men high in the Dark Triad spend extra effort enhancing their physical appearance, social image, and going out to talk with women. Men high in the Dark Triad even express a higher preference for living in cities over rural areas (where they have more potential victims, mates, and anonymity) (Jonason, 2018). Perhaps unsurprisingly, men high in the Dark Triad are also more likely to use dating apps like Tinder (Jonason & Bulyk, 2019), despite having lower mate value themselves, and more likely to post image-enhanced or edited selfies on social media (Fox & Rooney, 2015). The association between the Dark Triad and this kind of sexual competitiveness also holds across genders (Carter et al., 2015; Semenyna et al., 2019).

Aside from sexual coercion and assault, individuals high in the Dark Triad are more likely to break the “rules of love” in other ways as well. They are more likely to lie in attempts to enhance their perceived mate value (and to lie across the board) (Jonason et al., 2014). They are more likely to cheat or be sexually unfaithful to a partner (Jones & Weiser, 2014; Alavi et al., 2018; Sevi et al., 2020). High Dark Triad individuals are also more likely to engage in “mate poaching,” or trying to “steal” an already-partnered mate from someone else (Jonason et al., 2010; Brewer et al., 2015; Erik & Bhogal, 2016). Not only that, but individuals high in the Dark Triad are more likely to be poached — to be lured away from relationships they are already in — and more likely to be cheated on (Jonason et al., 2010; Brewer et al., 2015). It is unsurprising that Dark Triad traits are associated with divorce (Rabiee et al., 2023).

Finally it is important to note that people high in the Dark Triad mate assortatively: they pick one another (Smith et al., 2014; Webster et al., 2014; Birkas et al., 2018; Burtăverde, 2021; Kay, 2021). They view negative or antisocial traits in others as more attractive (Kornienko et al., 2020). They are also more likely to form friendships with others high in the Dark Triad, in order to help facilitate exploitative social strategies (Jonason & Schmitt, 2012). This results in lower average relationship satisfaction in general and further low satisfaction when there are certain dyadic combinations of Dark Triad traits (e.g. someone high in psychopathy with someone who is low, or when two individuals are both high in Machiavellianism) (Smith et al., 2014; Yücesan, 2016; Brewer & Abell, 2017; Goetz & Meyer, 2018; Kardum et al., 2018, 2023).

Reviewing the research on the Dark Triad we can paint a general picture: the high Dark Triad individual desires long-term relationships less, desires casual sex more, and spends more effort in attempts to acquire casual sex. They have a higher drive for sexual novelty and a higher willingness to use deception (and even force) to meet their sexual goals. When they do form long-term relationships they are less likely to be able to maintain them; their relationships are full of conflict and their relationships are more likely to end with the high Dark Triad individual cheating or being cheated on.

This doesn’t sound like a highly desirable mate! Yet it is easy to imagine how they accumulate more lifetime sexual partners.

How Dark Triad Individuals Acquire More Sexual Partners

Attractiveness and mating success are not the same thing, but given that the relationship between mate value and mating success is nonzero we might expect a relationship here. Holtzman & Strube (2011) proposed an evolutionary hypothesis for the evolution of short-term mating and narcissism. In short, narcissism evolved to co-vary with both physical attractiveness and coercive sexual strategies. Narcissists should be more physically attractive in order to effectively employ coercive short-term mating strategies.

Conversely, Campbell et al. (2006; Campbell & Foster, 2011) proposed an agentic model of narcissism and mating (Image 1). In this, personality traits associated with narcissism lead to self-perceptions and interpersonal behaviors that facilitate greater short-term mating success.

Here I refer to narcissism rather than the Dark Triad as a whole, because past research has also found that narcissism (but not psychopathy or Machiavellianism) is most closely associated with attractiveness and successful mating outcomes (Rauthmann & Kolar, 2013; Holtzman & Strube, 2010; Holtzman & Strube, 2013; Jauk et al., 2016; Salvino, 2018). Rather than the Dark Triad being attractive, narcissism specifically may be doing most of the heavy lifting.

Further, this may be specifically agentic narcissism rather than narcissistic rivalry (Dufner et al., 2013). The traits of narcissistic rivalry are antagonistic traits: hostility, high negative competitiveness, a tendency to devalue others, and increased interpersonal conflict (Rogoza et al., 2018). Narcissistic agency, on the other hand, is in effect high (or “narcissistic”) self-esteem (Campbell & Foster, 2011). Narcissistic self-esteem may be composed of inflated perceptions or value of the self, but is not as closely associated with negative interpersonal interactions. These are individuals who come across as charming, charismatic, extraverted, and exciting (Campbell 2006; Campbell & Foster, 2011). Individuals high in the Dark Triad rate themselves as more attractive and behave accordingly (Borráz-León & Rantala, 2021). 

Physical Attractiveness and the Dark Triad

Attractiveness may be physical or behavioral and per Holtzman & Strube (2010) we might expect to see correlations between physical attractiveness and the Dark Triad.

Past research has found that we can assess personality traits, including Dark Triad traits, in the face (Holtzman, 2011; Alper et al., 2021). This may remind you of physiognomy, or what has been called a “pseudoscience” of inferring character traits from facial characteristics. While it is true that a long, pointy nose isn’t indicative of telling lies, there is substantial support for the hypothesis that we can accurately discern some personality traits by a person’s appearance (Borkenau et al., 2009; Naumann et al., 2009; Kramer et al., 2011; Petrican, 2014; Walker & Vetter, 2016; Kachur et al., 2020). Two recent meta-analyses found modest effects of predicting personality traits and behaviors from facial photographs (Foo et al., 2022). This could be explained by assortative mating producing a covariability in desirability: because all traits are heritable, because we tend to prefer certain traits (both physical and behavioral) across cultures, and because we select assortatively for traits, we tend to see positive traits emerge as correlated within individuals (Conroy-Beam et al., 2019). We may also have adaptations to make heuristic decisions and identify the presence of such traits (Gigerenzer & Gaissmaier, 2011). For example, there is a bidirectional Halo Effect: we tend to perceive what is good as beautiful, what is beautiful as good, and process both in similar brain regions (see: The Neuroscience of Facial Attractiveness). 

One line of research has looked at the faces of individuals high and low in the Dark Triad. Faces of individuals high or low in the Dark Triad may be rated individually for attractiveness. Alternatively, faces of individuals high or low in the Dark Triad may be combined into facial morphs to create composites of High/Low Dark Triad faces.

This research has largely produced null or negative results. Table 1 summarizes research of the Dark Triad using facial ratings and facial morphs:

Lyons & Simeonov (2016) found null results and concluded, “It is possible that there are very little benefits for women in mating with high Dark Triad men, and that the self-reported mating success is based on effective manipulation of potential partners.” Similarly, Holtzman & Strube (2013) — originators of the hypothesis that narcissism was correlated with higher physical attractiveness — found null results when the unadorned full bodies of individuals high in the Dark Triad were rated. They concluded, “Due to the near-zero correlations between the Dark Triad and unadorned attractiveness in the current research, the evidence in our study is inconsistent with the prediction that dark personalities evolved to exhibit higher levels of unadorned attractiveness.”

The hypothesis that physical attractiveness and the Dark Triad covary is likely a wash. If anything, results are more consistent with our ability to detect dangerous traits in others and avoid them. Consistent with this, Gordon & Platek (2009) found higher activation in the amygdala — associated with danger and fear — when viewing faces high in the Dark Triad.

Behavioral Attractiveness and the Dark Triad

Given that there doesn’t seem to be a relationship between the Dark Triad and physical attractiveness, this leaves behavioral attractiveness on the table. Remember that there are two pathways to mating success and that they are not the same thing:

  1. Dark Trait traits are attractive to women (or men).
  2. Dark Triad traits facilitate behaviors in the actor that increase their own mating success.

Most of the research examining the behavioral attractiveness of the Dark Triad has used vignettes. A vignette is a short description or profile of an individual. These are invented by the researcher. Dark Triad vignettes are created by describing what a man high in the Dark Triad might be like. This is often done by taking items from a Dark Triad scale, such as the Dirty Dozen (Jonason & Webster, 2010) and rewriting them into the description of a person. Much of the commonly-cited research on the attractiveness of the Dark Triad has used vignettes. Table 2 summarizes these results:

These results are all over the place and also do not show a consistent picture of the Dark Triad being attractive — not even for short-term relationships.

Additional research on the attractiveness of the Dark Triad (that does not slot neatly into facial studies nor vignettes) is also inconsistent. In a speed-dating paradigm — where actual mate choice at zero acquaintance can be examined — Jauk et al. (2016) found that although Dark Triad traits were associated with rated mate appeal, they were not associated with men being more likely to be chosen for a future date. Haslam & Montrose (2015) selected 20 items from the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) and found that individuals rated high agreement with those statements as more attractive than low agreement. Dufner et al. (2013) gave fake forms of the NPI, with high or low scores, to participants and asked participants to rate the attractiveness of who they believed filled them out. The forms with high scores on the NPI were rated as more attractive than the forms with low scores. Further, individuals were asked to approach women on the street; men high in narcissism got more contact information (phone numbers). This effect was attributed to high physical attractiveness and “boldness” associated with agentic narcissism.

Replicating Carter et al. (2014)

Carter et al. (2014) conducted one of the early vignette studies assessing the attractiveness of the Dark Triad. In this they created two vignettes, one exhibiting high Dark Triad traits based on the Dirty Dozen (Jonason & Webster, 2010) and one exhibiting low Dark Triad traits. They sampled 128 female undergraduates (mean age 19) and asked them to rate the vignettes. In attractiveness, the High Dark Triad vignette was rated a 4.44 on a scale of 6 and the low DT trait was rated a 3.34.

I attempted to replicate this with a second convenience sample (N = 133 Female, mean age 36) using the methodology described in Carter et al. (2014). Below are the high and low Dark Triad vignettes:

“Richard is a hard worker and often feels highly motivated to seek prestige and status for himself. In his work and social life he is popular; he tends to want others to pay attention to him and occasionally uses flattery to get his way. Getting his own way in romantic relationships is important to Richard and he is not beyond manipulating his partners, using deceit, or lying to do so. He will occasionally exploit others toward his own ends and, when he does so, tends to lack remorse over his actions. If you were to ask him, he would say that he isn’t too concerned about the morality of his own actions.”

“Richard is a hard worker and often feels highly motivated to help others through his work. In his work and social life he is popular, although he doesn’t seek attention and is honest with others even when it is unflattering. Richard is willing to compromise in romantic relationships and tries hard to deal with romantic partners in an open and honest fashion. He has a strong moral compass, would feel remorse for intentionally harming others, and is unwilling to exploit or manipulate people for his own benefit. The morality of his own actions are important to Richard.”

Image 2 below shows the main result:

The analysis revealed a significant difference in attractiveness perception between the two conditions (t(130.05) = -35.01, p < .001). The mean attractiveness perception of the male vignette with high Dark Triad traits was substantially lower (M = 1.46) compared to the low Dark Triad vignette (M = 6.44).

This held both for short and long-term desirability (Images 3 and 4):

The results are wildly different — why?

Carter et al. (2014) did not describe the vignettes that they used, so the best I could do was to replicate the experiment following the description that they gave. Although the vignettes they used passed the manipulation check (participants rated the high Dark Triad vignette higher in Dark Triad traits than they did for the low Dark Triad vignette), ratings of Dark Triad traits were only moderate in Carter et al. (2014). In other words, their high Dark Triad vignettes were possibly written in such a way that they did not effectively communicate high Dark Triad traits. They communicated medium levels of Dark Triad traits.

Table 3 shows scores from Carter et al. (2014) and scores from the current study normalized on a scale of 10:

The high Dark Triad vignette I created was assessed as being higher across all Dark Triad traits than the vignette used in Carter et al. (2014) and the low Dark Triad vignette as lower across all Dark Triad traits.

Another potential explanation is age: the average age in my sample was 36 and in Carter et al. (2014) was 19. Do younger women show a higher preference for Dark Triad vignettes? Not in my sample. Age was not significantly correlated with attractiveness ratings for both high and low Dark Triad vignettes (r = -0.121, p = 0.351 High DT / r = -0.052, p = 0.665 Low DT).

Discussion

Given how robustly the Dark Triad is associated with poor relationship outcomes, it’s baffling that anyone would wish to emulate it. However, the Dark Triad has filtered into pop dating psychology as a Frakenstein of behaviors. I recently heard a call-in to a popular dating guru ask, to paraphrase, “I do MMA — how do I showcase my Dark Triad traits if I don’t fight in the ring?” Doing MMA has nothing at all to do with the Dark Triad, but likely does have something to do with behavioral dominance, fighting ability, athleticism, and winning physical contest competitions. People like this have probably formed an impression of the Dark Triad as a stereotypical “bad boy:” he wears leather jackets, rides a motorcycle, and probably smokes unfiltered cigarettes.

However, the Dark Triad is more closely associated with emotional instability and neuroticism (Lyons, 2019). Rather than being cool and collected, the Dark Triad individual is more likely to have an anxiety or depressive disorder (Birkás et al., 2016; Gómez-Leal et al., 2019). They have a higher fear of uncertainty, more anxious disturbances, and don’t even sleep as well at night because of it (Sabouri et al., 2016). Much of the “acting out” behavior of Dark Triad individuals reflects insecurity, defensiveness, and psychological immaturity (Richardson & Boag, 2016). It is more often the case that they present themselves as “virtuous victims” than men on top of the world (Ok et al., 2021). They are more likely to bully, but also more likely to be bullied themselves (Fatima, 2016; Pineda et al, 2022). Psychopathy, in particular, is associated with low status across measures (Spurk et al., 2016; Aluja et al., 2022). Youth high in the Dark Triad are more likely to become delinquent (Alsheikh Ali, 2020).

A recent meta-analysis found only the grandiose facet of narcissism may have some upsides for well-being (Blasco-Belled et al., 2024). Perhaps unsurprisingly, in the agentic model of Campbell & Foster (2011; see also Dufner et al., 2013) only agentic narcissism should predict higher attractiveness. Why? As we have seen, neither the Dark Triad nor narcissism specifically are linked with physical attractiveness. Further, Dark Triad traits are not probably not attractive in and of themselves.

Rather, individuals high in the Dark Triad are able to express or mimic prosocial traits and behaviors that would be attractive even in low Dark Triad individuals. These are, among other things, confidence, charm, charisma, and extraversion (Campbell, 2006; Campbell & Foster, 2011). This may even be a deliberate strategy. Brazil et al. (2023) found that individuals high in psychopathy were able to mimic prosocial traits in dating contexts in order to boost their own attractiveness.

Mimicry behavior is typical of antisocial individuals, from Cluster B disorders like antisocial personality disorder (ASPD), to psychopathy, to the Dark Triad (Book, 2015; de Roos et al., 2022). They mirror the expectations of peers and potential mates. They try to emulate individuals who have the traits that they do not. This is occasionally effective, but more often disastrous (especially in the long-term), which is part of why high Dark Triad individuals cannot maintain lasting romantic relationships.

Not one single study on the Dark Triad has uncovered that “being a jerk” is what drives its associations with mating success. Your goal should not be to mimic Dark Triad traits — it should be to cultivate the authentic, prosocial traits associated with attractiveness that Dark Triad individuals will only ever be able to mimic. Further, if you want satisfying long-term relationships with high quality women who will not cheat on you then you should lean as far away from Dark Triad behaviors as you are able to. Refer back to the introduction of this article and the principle of assortative mating: individuals high in the Dark Triad select mates who are as well. At the end of the day the partners you attract will be similar to you.

References

Alavi, M., Mei, T. K., & Mehrinezhad, S. A. (2018). The Dark Triad of personality and infidelity intentions: The moderating role of relationship experience. Personality and Individual Differences, 128, 49-54.

Alsheikh Ali, A. S. A. (2020). Delinquency as predicted by dark triad factors and demographic variables. International journal of adolescence and youth, 25(1), 661-675.

Alper, S., Bayrak, F., & Yilmaz, O. (2021). All the Dark Triad and some of the Big Five traits are visible in the face. Personality and Individual Differences, 168, 110350.

Aluja, A., Garcia, L. F., Rossier, J., Ostendorf, F., Glicksohn, J., Oumar, B., … & Hansenne, M. (2022). Dark triad traits, social position, and personality: a cross-cultural study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 53(3-4), 380-402.

Blasco‐Belled, A., Tejada‐Gallardo, C., Alsinet, C., & Rogoza, R. (2024). The links of subjective and psychological well‐being with the Dark Triad traits: A meta‐analysis. Journal of Personality, 92(2), 584-600.

Birkás, B., Láng, A., Martin, L., & Kállai, J. (2016). Disturbing concerns for dark personalities: anxiety sensitivity and the Dark Triad. International Journal of Advances in Psychology, 5(1), 1-5.

Birkás, B., Láng, A., & Meskó, N. (2018). Self-rated attractiveness moderates the relationship between dark personality traits and romantic ideals in women. Psychological Reports, 121(1), 184-200.

Book, A., Methot, T., Gauthier, N., Hosker-Field, A., Forth, A., Quinsey, V., & Molnar, D. (2015). The mask of sanity revisited: Psychopathic traits and affective mimicry. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 1(2), 91-102.

Borkenau, P., Brecke, S., Möttig, C., & Paelecke, M. (2009). Extraversion is accurately perceived after a 50-ms exposure to a face. Journal of research in personality, 43(4), 703-706.

Borráz-León, J. I., & Rantala, M. J. (2021). Does the Dark Triad predict self-perceived attractiveness, mate value, and number of sexual partners both in men and women?. Personality and Individual Differences, 168, 110341.

Brazil, K. J., & Forth, A. E. (2020). Psychopathy and the induction of desire: Formulating and testing an evolutionary hypothesis. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 6(1), 64-81.

Brazil, K. J., Cloutier, D., De Las Llagas, N., McMahon, S. G., Benevides, V., Book, A. S., & Visser, B. A. (2023). The chameleons of dating: psychopathic traits are associated with mimicking prosocial personality traits in dating contexts. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 9(2), 260-269.

Brewer, G., Hunt, D., James, G., & Abell, L. (2015). Dark Triad traits, infidelity and romantic revenge. Personality and Individual Differences, 83, 122-127.

Brewer, G., & Abell, L. (2017). Machiavellianism, relationship satisfaction, and romantic relationship quality. Europe’s journal of psychology, 13(3), 491.

Brewer, G., Carter, G. L., Lyons, M., & Green, J. (2018). Sensation-seeking in women does not affect their preference for Dark Triad male faces. Personality and Individual Differences, 130, 92-95.

Butković, A., Vatavuk, K., & Wertag, A. (2021). HEXACO and Dark Triad personality traits as predictors of male attractiveness in different relationship contexts. Psychological Topics, 30(2), 313-326.

Burtăverde, V. (2021). Women high on the Dark Triad traits are more attracted to narcissistic males if they are oriented to long term mating and had fewer experiences with unfaithful men. Personality and Individual Differences, 173, 110627.

Burtăverde, V., Jonason, P. K., Ene, C., & Istrate, M. (2021). On being “dark” and promiscuous: The Dark Triad traits, mate value, disgust, and sociosexuality. Personality and Individual Differences, 168, 110255.

Buss, D. M. (1988). The evolution of human intrasexual competition: tactics of mate attraction. Journal of personality and social psychology, 54(4), 616.

Campbell, W. K., Brunell, A. B., & Finkel, E. J. (2006). Narcissism, Interpersonal Self-Regulation, and Romantic Relationships: An Agency Model Approach.

Campbell, W. K., & Foster, J. D. (2011). The narcissistic self: Background, an extended agency model, and ongoing controversies. In The self (pp. 115-138). Psychology Press.

Carter, G. L., Campbell, A. C., & Muncer, S. (2014). The Dark Triad: Beyond a ‘male’mating strategy. Personality and Individual Differences, 56, 159-164.

Carter, G. L., Campbell, A. C., & Muncer, S. (2014). The dark triad personality: Attractiveness to women. Personality and Individual Differences, 56, 57-61.

Carter, G. L., Montanaro, Z., Linney, C., & Campbell, A. C. (2015). Women’s sexual competition and the Dark Triad. Personality and Individual Differences, 74, 275-279.

Conroy-Beam, D., Roney, J. R., Lukaszewski, A. W., Buss, D. M., Asao, K., Sorokowska, A., … & Zupančič, M. (2019). Assortative mating and the evolution of desirability covariation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 40(5), 479-491.

de Roos, M. S., & Jones, D. N. (2022). Assessing deception differences with mimicry deception theory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 104(1), 44-56.

Demuthova, S., Demuth, A., & Spasovski, O. The Dark Triad in Faces: The Influence of Personality Characteristics and Gender on Perceived Attractiveness.

Demuthova, S., & Demuth, A. (2023). The Attractiveness of Dark Triad Characteristics in Facial Composites with Respect to Gender. Sexuality and Gender Studies Journal, 1(2), 1-10.

Dragostinov, Y., & Booth, T. (2023). Is the Dark Triad Desirable? Evaluating the Attractiveness of Fictitious Characters for Short-and Long-Term Relationships.

Dufner, M., Rauthmann, J. F., Czarna, A. Z., & Denissen, J. J. (2013). Are narcissists sexy? Zeroing in on the effect of narcissism on short-term mate appeal. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39(7), 870-882.

Erik, E., & Bhogal, M. S. (2016). Do the dark triad and self-perceived mate value predict intention to mate poach?.

Fatima, I. (2016). Dark Triad Personality Traits as Predictors of Bullying and Victimization in Adolescents. Journal of Behavioural Sciences, 26(1).

Figueredo, A. J., Gladden, P. R., Sisco, M. M., Patch, E. A., & Jones, D. N. (2015). The unholy trinity: The Dark Triad, sexual coercion, and Brunswik-symmetry. Evolutionary Psychology, 13(2), 147470491501300208.

Foo, Y. Z., Sutherland, C. A. M., Burton, N. S., Nakagawa, S., & Rhodes, G. (2022). Accuracy in facial trustworthiness impressions: Kernel of truth or modern physiognomy? A meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 48(11), 1580-1596.

Fox, J., & Rooney, M. C. (2015). The Dark Triad and trait self-objectification as predictors of men’s use and self-presentation behaviors on social networking sites. Personality and Individual Differences, 76, 161-165.

Gigerenzer, G., & Gaissmaier, W. (2011). Heuristic decision making. Annual review of psychology, 62, 451-482.

Goetz, C. D., & Meyer, K. B. (2018). Mate value discrepancies, the dark triad and relationship satisfaction: a Euclidean distances approach. Evolutionary psychological science, 4, 134-140.

Gómez-Leal, R., Megías-Robles, A., Gutiérrez-Cobo, M. J., Cabello, R., Fernández-Abascal, E. G., & Fernández-Berrocal, P. (2019). Relationship between the Dark Triad and depressive symptoms. PeerJ, 7, e8120.

Gordon, D. S., & Platek, S. M. (2009). Trustworthy? The brain knows: Implicit neural responses to faces that vary in dark triad personality characteristics and trustworthiness. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 3(3), 182.

Haslam, C., & Montrose, V. T. (2015). Should have known better: The impact of mating experience and the desire for marriage upon attraction to the narcissistic personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 82, 188–192.

Holtzman, N. S., & Strube, M. J. (2010). Narcissism and attractiveness. Journal of Research in Personality, 44(1), 133-136.

Holtzman, N. S., & Strube, M. J. (2011). The intertwined evolution of narcissism and short‐term mating: An emerging hypothesis. The handbook of narcissism and narcissistic personality disorder: Theoretical approaches, empirical findings, and treatments, 210-220.

Holtzman, N. S. (2011). Facing a psychopath: Detecting the dark triad from emotionally-neutral faces, using prototypes from the Personality Faceaurus. Journal of Research in Personality, 45(6), 648-654.

Holtzman, N. S., & Strube, M. J. (2013). People with dark personalities tend to create a physically attractive veneer. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4(4), 461-467.

Jauk, E., Neubauer, A. C., Mairunteregger, T., Pemp, S., Sieber, K. P., & Rauthmann, J. F. (2016). How alluring are dark personalities? The dark triad and attractiveness in speed dating. European Journal of Personality, 30(2), 125-138.

Jonason, P. K., Li, N. P., Webster, G. D., & Schmitt, D. P. (2009). The dark triad: Facilitating a short‐term mating strategy in men. European Journal of Personality: Published for the European Association of Personality Psychology, 23(1), 5-18.

Jonason, P. K., & Webster, G. D. (2010). The dirty dozen: a concise measure of the dark triad. Psychological assessment, 22(2), 420.

Jonason, P. K., Koenig, B. L., & Tost, J. (2010). Living a fast life: The Dark Triad and life history theory. Human Nature, 21, 428-442.

Jonason, P. K., Li, N. P., & Buss, D. M. (2010). The costs and benefits of the Dark Triad: Implications for mate poaching and mate retention tactics. Personality and individual differences, 48(4), 373-378.

Jonason, P. K., Valentine, K. A., Li, N. P., & Harbeson, C. L. (2011). Mate-selection and the Dark Triad: Facilitating a short-term mating strategy and creating a volatile environment. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(6), 759-763.

Jonason, P. K., & Schmitt, D. P. (2012). What have you done for me lately? Friendship-selection in the shadow of the Dark Triad traits. Evolutionary Psychology, 10(3), 147470491201000303.

Jonason, P. K., Luevano, V. X., & Adams, H. M. (2012). How the Dark Triad traits predict relationship choices. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(3), 180-184.

Jonason, P. K., & Webster, G. D. (2012). A protean approach to social influence: Dark Triad personalities and social influence tactics. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(4), 521-526.

Jonason, P. K., Lyons, M., Baughman, H. M., & Vernon, P. A. (2014). What a tangled web we weave: The Dark Triad traits and deception. Personality and Individual Differences, 70, 117-119.

Jonason, P. K., Lyons, M., & Blanchard, A. (2015). Birds of a “bad” feather flock together: The Dark Triad and mate choice. Personality and Individual Differences, 78, 34-38.

Jonason, P. K., Foster, J. D., Egorova, M. S., Parshikova, O., Csathó, Á., Oshio, A., & Gouveia, V. V. (2017). The dark triad traits from a life history perspective in six countries. Frontiers in psychology, 8, 1476.

Jonason, P. K., Girgis, M., & Milne-Home, J. (2017). The exploitive mating strategy of the Dark Triad traits: Tests of rape-enabling attitudes. Archives of sexual behavior, 46, 697-706.

Jonason, P. K. (2018). Bright lights, big city: The Dark Triad traits and geographical preferences. Personality and Individual Differences, 132, 66-73.

Jonason, P., & Bulyk, R. (2019). Who uses Tinder?: The Dark Triad traits, attachment, and mate value. Studia Psychologica, 19(1).

Jonason, P. K., White, K. P., & Al-Shawaf, L. (2020). Should I stay or should I go: Individual differences in response to romantic dealmakers and dealbreakers. Personality and Individual Differences, 164, 110120.

Jones, D. N., & Weiser, D. A. (2014). Differential infidelity patterns among the Dark Triad. Personality and Individual differences, 57, 20-24.

Kachur, A., Osin, E., Davydov, D., Shutilov, K., & Novokshonov, A. (2020). Assessing the Big Five personality traits using real-life static facial images. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 8487.

Kardum, I., Hudek-Knezevic, J., Mehic, N., & Pilek, M. (2018). The effects of similarity in the dark triad traits on the relationship quality in dating couples. Personality and individual differences, 131, 38-44.

Kardum, I., Hudek‐Knezevic, J., Mehić, N., & Banov Trošelj, K. (2023). The dark triad traits and relationship satisfaction: Dyadic response surface analysis. Journal of personality.

Kay, C. S. (2021). Negative traits, positive assortment: Revisiting the dark triad and a preference for similar others. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 38(4), 1259-1278.

Koladich, S. J., & Atkinson, B. E. (2016). The dark triad and relationship preferences: A replication and extension. Personality and Individual Differences, 94, 253-255.

Kornienko, D. S., Derish, F. V., S Kornienko, D., & V Derish, F. The Dark Triad Predicts Attitudes Towards Negative Personality Traits. European Proceedings of Social and Behavioural Sciences, 94.

Kramer, R. S., King, J. E., & Ward, R. (2011). Identifying personality from the static, nonexpressive face in humans and chimpanzees: Evidence of a shared system for signaling personality. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(3), 179-185.

Lyons, M. T., Marcinkowska, U. M., Helle, S., & McGrath, L. (2015). Mirror, mirror, on the wall, who is the most masculine of them all? The Dark Triad, masculinity, and women’s mate choice. Personality and Individual Differences, 74, 153-158.

Lyons, M., & Simeonov, L. (2016). The undesirable Dark Triad? Women dislike Dark Triad male faces across different mating context and socio-ecological conditions. Personality and Individual Differences, 90, 338-341.

Lyons, M., & Blanchard, A. (2016). “I could see, in the depth of his eyes, my own beauty reflected”: Women’s assortative preference for narcissistic, but not for Machiavellian or psychopathic male faces. Personality and individual differences, 97, 40-44.

Lyons, M. (2019). The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, machiavellianism, and psychopathy in everyday life. Academic Press.

Marcinkowska, U. M., Helle, S., & Lyons, M. T. (2015). Dark traits: Sometimes hot, and sometimes not? Female preferences for Dark Triad faces depend on sociosexuality and contraceptive use. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 369-373.

McDonald, M. M., Donnellan, M. B., & Navarrete, C. D. (2012). A life history approach to understanding the Dark Triad. Personality and individual differences, 52(5), 601-605.

Monteiro, R. P., Lopes, G. S., Nascimento, B. S., Gouveia, V. V., Shackelford, T. K., & Zeigler-Hill, V. (2017). Dark Triad predicts self-promoting mate attraction behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences, 119, 83-85.

Navas, M. P., Maneiro, L., Cutrín, O., Gómez-Fraguela, J. A., & Sobral, J. (2022). Sexism, moral disengagement, and dark triad traits on perpetrators of sexual violence against women and community men. Sexual Abuse, 34(7), 857-884.

Naumann, L. P., Vazire, S., Rentfrow, P. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2009). Personality judgments based on physical appearance. Personality and social psychology bulletin, 35(12), 1661-1671.

Ok, E., Qian, Y., Strejcek, B., & Aquino, K. (2021). Signaling virtuous victimhood as indicators of Dark Triad personalities. Journal of personality and social psychology, 120(6), 1634.

Pavlović, T., Markotić, A., & Bartolin, A. (2019). Dark Triad and estimated probability of sexual coercion. Personality and individual differences, 151, 109527.

Petrican, R., Todorov, A., & Grady, C. (2014). Personality at face value: Facial appearance predicts self and other personality judgments among strangers and spouses. Journal of nonverbal behavior, 38, 259-277.

Pineda, D., Rico-Bordera, P., Martínez-Martínez, A., Galán, M., & Piqueras, J. A. (2022). Dark tetrad personality traits also play a role in bullying victimization. Frontiers in psychology, 13, 984744.

Prusik, M., Konopka, K., & Kocur, D. (2021). Too many shades of gray: The Dark Triad and its linkage to coercive and coaxing tactics to obtain sex and the quality of romantic relationships. Personality and Individual Differences, 170, 110413.

Qureshi, C., Harris, E., & Atkinson, B. E. (2016). Relationships between age of females and attraction to the Dark Triad personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 95, 200-203.

Rabiee, N., Koochakentezar, R., Ghanbari Panah, A., & Kooshki, S. (2023). Predicting marital commitment based on dark triad traits and big five of personality. Journal of Psychological Science, 22(126), 1103-1120.

Rauthmann, J. F., & Kolar, G. P. (2013). The perceived attractiveness and traits of the Dark Triad: Narcissists are perceived as hot, Machiavellians and psychopaths not. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(5), 582-586.

Rogoza, R., Żemojtel-Piotrowska, M., Kwiatkowska, M. M., & Kwiatkowska, K. (2018). The bright, the dark, and the blue face of narcissism: the spectrum of narcissism in its relations to the metatraits of personality, self-esteem, and the nomological network of shyness, loneliness, and empathy. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 328535.

Richardson, E. N., & Boag, S. (2016). Offensive defenses: The mind beneath the mask of the dark triad traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 92, 148-152.

Sabouri, S., Gerber, M., Lemola, S., Becker, S. P., Shamsi, M., Shakouri, Z., … & Brand, S. (2016). Examining Dark Triad traits in relation to sleep disturbances, anxiety sensitivity and intolerance of uncertainty in young adults. Comprehensive psychiatry, 68, 103-110.

Salvino, M. A. (2018). Why do we desire ‘dark’ personalities? A study investigating attractiveness of the Dark Triad personality traits.

Schröder–Abé, M., Rentzsch, K., Asendorpf, J. B., & Penke, L. (2016). Good Enough for An Affair. Self–Enhancement of Attractiveness, Interest in Potential Mates and Popularity as A Mate. European Journal of Personality, 30(1), 12-18.

Semenyna, S. W., Vasey, P. L., & Honey, P. L. (2019). Replicating the relationships between Dark Triad traits and female mate-competition tactics in undergraduate women. Personality and Individual Differences, 147, 73-78.

Sevi, B., Urganci, B., & Sakman, E. (2020). Who cheats? An examination of light and dark personality traits as predictors of infidelity. Personality and Individual Differences, 164, 110126.

Smith, C. V., Hadden, B. W., Webster, G. D., Jonason, P. K., Gesselman, A. N., & Crysel, L. C. (2014). Mutually attracted or repulsed? Actor–partner interdependence models of Dark Triad traits and relationship outcomes. Personality and Individual Differences, 67, 35-41.

Solomon, E., & Lyons, M. (2020). Not my protector—Women have an aversion to high Dark Triad faces irrespective of childhood or current environmental danger. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 6, 241-245.

Spurk, D., Keller, A. C., & Hirschi, A. (2016). Do bad guys get ahead or fall behind? Relationships of the dark triad of personality with objective and subjective career success. Social psychological and personality science, 7(2), 113-121.

Thornhill, R., & Palmer, C. T. (2001). A natural history of rape: Biological bases of sexual coercion. MIT press.

Valentova, J. V., Junior, F. P. M., Štěrbová, Z., Varella, M. A. C., & Fisher, M. L. (2020). The association between Dark Triad traits and sociosexuality with mating and parenting efforts: A cross-cultural study. Personality and Individual Differences, 154, 109613.

Walker, M., & Vetter, T. (2016). Changing the personality of a face: Perceived Big Two and Big Five personality factors modeled in real photographs. Journal of personality and social psychology, 110(4), 609.

Weber, S., Geukes, K., Leckelt, M., & Back, M. D. (2021). The attractiveness of narcissists: Hard work or natural beauty?. Self and Identity, 20(2), 235-267.

Webster, G. D., Gesselman, A. N., Crysel, L. C., Brunell, A. B., & Jonason, P. K. (2014). An actor—Partner interdependence model of the Dark Triad and aggression in couples. Personality and Individual Differences, 60, S16.

Westhead, J., & Egan, V. (2015). Untangling the concurrent influences of the Dark Triad, personality and mating effort on violence. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 222-226.

Zeigler-Hill, V., Besser, A., Morag, J., & Campbell, W. K. (2016). The Dark Triad and sexual harassment proclivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 89, 47-54.

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

You May Also Like